Log InSign Up
L
6 min read

Language

Published: Jul 17, 2023
  /  
Updated: Jul 28, 2023

Written by Oseh Mathias

Founder, SpeechFit

Language is a unique and complex human capacity that allows us to communicate and convey meaning through a system of symbols, sounds, and rules[1]. It encompasses both spoken and written forms and serves as a fundamental aspect of human culture and social interaction[2].

Language can be broken down into various components, and understanding these components is crucial for speech-language pathologists (SLPs) in their practice. The main components include:

  1. Phonology: This refers to the system of sounds in a language and the rules for combining them[3]. Phonological disorders involve difficulty understanding or using this system, which can impact a person's ability to form intelligible speech[4].

  2. Morphology: This concerns the structure of words, which are built from smaller units of meaning known as morphemes (e.g., 'unhappiness' comprises the morphemes 'un-', '-happy', and '-ness')[5]. Morphological disorders can make it challenging for individuals to form or understand complex words[6].

  3. Syntax: The set of rules for combining words into phrases and sentences[7]. Someone with a syntactic disorder may struggle to form grammatically correct sentences or understand complex sentence structures[8].

  4. Semantics: The meaning of words and sentences[9]. Semantic disorders can manifest as difficulties understanding or using vocabulary appropriately[10].

  5. Pragmatics: The rules and conventions for using language in a social context, including turn-taking, maintaining a topic during conversation, and using appropriate body language and tone[11]. Pragmatic disorders can lead to difficulties with social communication[12].

Speech-language pathologists diagnose and treat disorders in any of these components of language[13]. They may work with clients who have developmental disorders (such as autism or Down syndrome), acquired conditions (like stroke or traumatic brain injury), or progressive neurological conditions (such as Parkinson's disease or dementia)[14]. SLPs use a variety of techniques to help individuals improve their language skills, which might involve direct therapy, using augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) systems, caregiver training, or environmental adaptations[15].

Language is not just the words we speak. It also includes non-verbal communication such as gestures, facial expressions, and body language[16]. These aspects are especially important when working with individuals who have severe speech and language difficulties, as they may rely more heavily on non-verbal forms of communication[17].

The origins of language are hard to pinpoint as the development predates written history. It's largely believed that language originated as a form of nonverbal communication and gradually evolved into more complex forms over hundreds of thousands of years[18].

  1. Primate Communication (40 - 50 million years ago): Non-human primates communicate through gestures, facial expressions, and vocalizations, which likely represent the origins of human language[19].

  2. Homo habilis/Homo erectus (2.3 million - 1.4 million years ago): The first Homo species developed primitive tools and likely had a simple form of communication, but it's uncertain if they used a fully developed language[20].

  3. Homo heidelbergensis/Neanderthals (600,000 - 30,000 years ago): These human relatives had a complex culture and likely used some form of language to communicate. This belief is bolstered by the discovery of symbols and cave paintings[21].

  4. Homo sapiens (300,000 years ago - Present): The development of fully modern language likely coincided with the emergence of Homo sapiens. By 50,000 years ago, humans were creating sophisticated art and tools, evidence of complex symbolic thought that suggests the existence of a complex language[22].

Dialects are regional or social variations of a language. They emerge when populations become geographically or socially isolated from each other, leading to slight differences in pronunciation, grammar, or vocabulary that accumulate over time[23]. English has many dialects due to the spread of the British Empire and its subsequent influence over many parts of the world[24].

  1. Old English (5th - 12th Century): Influenced by the Germanic tribes (Angles, Saxons, Jutes) who invaded Britain[25].

  2. Middle English (12th - 15th Century): A period of significant linguistic change, influenced by the Norman Conquest[26].

  3. Early Modern English (15th - 17th Century): This era witnessed the Great Vowel Shift, a major change in pronunciation. The King James Bible and Shakespeare's works date from this period[27].

  4. Late Modern English (18th Century - Present): The English we speak today. Driven by the Industrial Revolution, scientific advancements, British colonialism, and American influence, the language has continued to absorb foreign words and evolve[28].

One fascinating case of language involves the whistle languages, such as Silbo used by the inhabitants of some of the Canary Islands, including La Gomera and El Hierro. This language is composed entirely of whistling sounds, which are used to replicate the phonetics of the local spoken language (Spanish in this case). Whistle languages can be heard over great distances, making them effective for communication in rugged, mountainous terrain or densely forested regions[29]. There are more than 80 whistled speech languages around the world, mostly coinciding with mountainous and forested regions.

image within the content - in line image
There are more than 80 known whistled languages around the world, usually coinciding with dense vegetation and rugged terrain. Meyer, J. (2021). [30]

Additionally, some indigenous communities, such as the Aboriginal people of Australia, have sophisticated oral traditions that transmit cultural knowledge and history through storytelling. These practices demonstrate the power of human communication systems beyond spoken or written language[31].

Language processing involves a complex network of areas in the brain. The two most famous regions are Broca's area and Wernicke's area, named after the scientists who discovered them.

  • Broca's Area: Located in the left frontal lobe, Broca's area is primarily involved in language production and speech fluency. It plays a crucial role in the planning and coordination of the motor movements required for articulate speech[32].

  • Wernicke's Area: Situated in the left temporal lobe, Wernicke's area is primarily associated with language comprehension. It helps decode and understand spoken and written language[33].

  • Arcuate Fasciculus: This bundle of nerve fibers connects Broca's area and Wernicke's area, facilitating the exchange of information between language production and comprehension centers. It is involved in the integration of language-related processes[34].

  • Auditory Cortex: Found in the temporal lobes, the auditory cortex processes and analyzes auditory information, including spoken language sounds. It helps to decode speech sounds into meaningful linguistic units[35].

  • Visual Cortex: Visual regions in the occipital lobes contribute to language processing by aiding in the recognition and comprehension of written language[36].

  • Angular Gyrus: Situated at the junction of the temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes, the angular gyrus plays a role in semantic processing, linking visual input and language comprehension[37]. This area is sometimes referred to as Geschwind's Territory.

  • Supramarginal Gyrus: Located in the parietal lobe, the supramarginal gyrus supports language-related functions such as phonological processing and working memory for speech[38].

  • Prefrontal Cortex: The prefrontal cortex, particularly the left hemisphere, is involved in higher-level language functions such as language monitoring, self-regulation, and executive control[39].

These brain regions work together as a distributed network to support different aspects of language processing, including production, comprehension, semantic understanding, and integration of auditory and visual information[40].


Author

Oseh Mathias

SpeechFit Founder

Oseh is passionate about improving health and wellbeing outcomes for neurodiverse people and healthcare providers alike.


References
  • Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York, NY: Harper Perennial Modern Classics.

  • Hall, S. (1997). Representation: Cultural Representations and Signifying Practices. London, UK: SAGE Publications Ltd.

  • Ladefoged, P. (2006). A Course in Phonetics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Thomson Wadsworth.

  • Shriberg, L. D., & Kent, R. D. (2013). Clinical Phonetics (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

  • Aronoff, M., & Fudeman, K. (2010). What is Morphology? (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with Specific Language Impairment (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Fromkin, V., Rodman, R., & Hyams, N. (2013). An Introduction to Language (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

  • Cruse, D. A. (2006). A Glossary of Semantics and Pragmatics. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.

  • Capone Singleton, N. (2018). Language and Literacy Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. In F. Volkmar (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York, NY: Springer.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The Essential Difference: Male and Female Brains and the Truth About Autism. New York, NY: Basic Books.

  • Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner Review: The Assessment of Language Pragmatics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(8), 973-987.

  • Paul, R., & Norbury, C. (2012). Language Disorders from Infancy through Adolescence: Listening, Speaking, Reading, Writing, and Communicating (4th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Elsevier.

  • Murray, L. L., Chapey, R., & Lender, J. (2011). Introduction to Speech-Language Pathology and its Disorders. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language Intervention Strategies in Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders (5th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

  • Beukelman, D., & Mirenda, P. (2013). Augmentative and Alternative Communication: Management of Severe Communication Disorders in Children and Adults (4th ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.

  • Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  • McCann, J., & Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Critical Review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 325-350.

  • Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. W. W. Norton & Company.

  • Pollick, A. S., & de Waal, F. B. (2007). Ape gestures and language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(19), 8184-8189.

  • Rightmire, G. P. (2009). Homo in the middle pleistocene: Hypodigms, variation, and species recognition. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(1), 8-21.

  • Zilhão, J. (2011). Aliens from Outer Time? Why the “Human Revolution” Is Wrong, and Where Do We Go from Here?. Human Origins, 31-56.

  • Tattersall, I. (2012). Masters of the planet: the search for our human origins. Macmillan.

  • Chambers, J. K., & Trudgill, P. (1998). Dialectology (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.

  • Mesthrie, R. (2008). The spread of English in South Africa: 1856–1925. World Englishes, 27(3), 341-364.

  • Hogg, R. M. (1992). A grammar of Old English, Vol. 1: Phonology. Blackwell.

  • Baugh, A. C., & Cable, T. (2002). A history of the English language (5th ed.). Routledge.

  • Lass, R. (1999). Change and continuity in English inflection. In The development of standard English, 1300-1800 (pp. 32-62). Cambridge University Press.

  • Crystal, D. (2005). The Stories of English. The Overlook Press.

  • Trujillo, R. P. (1978). El silbo gomero: análisis lingüístico (Vol. 9). Universidad de La Laguna, Secretariado de Publicaciones.

  • Meyer, J. (2021). Studying Whistled Languages. Smithsonian Magazine. Retrieved from https://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/studying-whistled-languages-180978484/

  • Muecke, S., & Curran, G. (2021). Indigenous Oral Traditions in Anthropology. Anthropological Forum, 31(1), 67-82.

  • Grodzinsky, Y., & Santi, A. (2008). The battle for Broca's region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(12), 474-480.

  • Binder, J. R. (2015). The Wernicke area: Modern evidence and a reinterpretation. Neurology, 85(24), 2170-2175.

  • Dick, A. S., & Tremblay, P. (2012). Beyond the arcuate fasciculus: consensus and controversy in the connectional anatomy of language. Brain, 135(12), 3529-3550.

  • Mesgarani, N., Cheung, C., Johnson, K., & Chang, E. F. (2014). Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science, 343(6174), 1006-1010.

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254-262.

  • Seghier, M. L. (2013). The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. The Neuroscientist, 19(1), 43-61.

  • Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 62-88.

  • Badre, D., & D'Esposito, M. (2009). Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(9), 659-669.

  • Hagoort, P. (2014). Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: Broca's region and beyond. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 136-141.